Silberstein Presentation

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    1/38

    Relationships Among

    Emotional-Schemas,Psychological Flexibility,

    Dispositional Mindfulness, &

    Emotion Regulation in AdultCognitive-Behavioral Therapy

    Outpatients

    Laura R. Silberstein PsyD,Dennis D. Tirch PhD & Robert L. Leahy PhD

    American Institute for Cognitive Therapy

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    2/38

    Introduction

    Mindfulness, psychological flexibility, and emotional schemas

    have each been related to emotional experiencing and

    responding, as well as to the alleviation of human suffering(Corrigan, 2004; Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, and Strosahl, 1996; Martin, 1997; Fulton and

    Seigel, 2005).

    Mindfulness, acceptance and emotional schemas have each

    been associated with challenging experiential avoidance and

    contributing to emotion regulation.

    This study focuses on the relationships betweenemotional schemas, psychological flexibility and

    dispositional mindfulness.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    3/38

    Mindfulness

    The meta-experiential concept of mindfulness refers to a mode of

    perception or awareness of the present moment in a nonjudgmental

    and accepting manner (Kabat-Zinn, 1990).

    Dispositional mindfulness is an innate characteristic reflecting an

    individuals natural occurring ability to inhabit this intentional stance

    of awareness (Brown and Ryan, 2003).

    Mindfulness offers a perceptual, rather than cognitive or affective

    presentation of the current moment as it is.

    Dispositional mindfulness has been associated with an increased

    capacity to let go of negative thoughts and is viewed as a core

    process in psychological flexibility (Frewen, Evans, Maraj, Dozois, and Partridge,2008; Hayes, Strosahl and Wilson, 1999).

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    4/38

    Psychological Flexibility

    Psychological f lexibil i tyhas been defined as the ability tofully encounter an experience without gratuitous defense and,depending upon the context, continuing or changing behaviorin the pursuit of goals and values (Hayes et al., 2006).

    The term psychological flexibility has been used as a generalfactor to provide a more specific description of the core

    processes involved in experiential avoidance and experientialacceptance (Hayes et al., 2006).

    Experiential avoidance and acceptance are subsumed bypsychological flexibility, although they are still useful ways todescribe aspects of this construct(Bond, et al., In press).

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    5/38

    Psychological Flexibility

    Experiential acceptance is the practice of just letting things beand experiencing them as they are. It involves being open toan experience and willing to remain in contact with it, even ifthe experience is unpleasant (Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, and Hofman, 2006).

    Acceptance can be described as second-order change or meta-change and is often viewed as preceding behavior or thoughtchanges (Germer, 2005; Hayes, 2001).

    The focus is on changing the influence of the thoughts and

    emotions by shifting ones response to them and not trying tochange or struggle with the thoughts or feelings themselves(Bishop et al., 2004).

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    6/38

    Psychological Flexibility

    Exper ienti al avoidanceresults when an individual is unwilling toremain in contact with a particular experience and attempts to alterthe form or frequency of these experiences (Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, andStrosahl, 1996).

    Avoidance can be considered a broad category or class of behaviors

    that attempt to escape, avoid or modify a particular internal orexternal stimulus (Chapman, Gratz, and Brown, 2006).

    Individuals who engage in experiential avoidance often engage inmore control tactics and verbal strategies to regulate their emotions(Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, and Hofman, 2006; Cochrane, Barnes-Holmes, Stewart, andLuciano, 2007).

    The negative effects of experiential avoidance result from theineffective and often paradoxical process of attempts to avoid or alter

    private events (Wenzlaff and Wegner, 2000).

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    7/38

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    8/38

    Emotional Schemas

    Individuals vary in their perception of emotions as temporary,universal, comprehensible, complex experiencesthat can besubject to acceptance, validation, and expression (Leahy 2007).

    Emotional schema dimensions range from flexible and

    adaptive to more rigid and maladaptive.

    This model of emotional schemas contends that those whoendorse maladaptive emotional schemas are more likely toresist certain emotions and engage in avoidant strategies (Leahy2002).

    Also emphasizes the positive implications in overcomingavoidance, increasing emotional processing, and the role ofcognition in emotional experiencing (Leahy 2007).

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    9/38

    Present Study Rationale

    The current study aims to explore the relationships between

    emotional schemas, psychological flexibility and

    dispositional mindfulness.

    This research also examines the relative contribution of

    mindful awareness and emotional schemas to

    psychological flexibility.

    The roles of dispositional mindfulness and those emotional

    schemas which represent emotion regulation strategies will

    be explored in terms of their relationship with psychological

    flexibility.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    10/38

    Current Study

    The current research is a cross sectional, observational

    study of current patients at the American Institute for

    Cognitive Therapy.

    202 patients volunteered to complete three brief

    measures assessing dispositional mindfulness,

    psychological flexibility, and emotional schemas. The

    responses are largely from intake assessments, typically

    collected at the beginning of therapy.

    The cognitive-behavioral therapy offered at this institute

    primarily was non-manualized and included elements of

    Beckian Cognitive Therapy and integrated with elements

    of ACT, DBT, and Buddhist Psychology.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    11/38

    Measures

    Mindfulness Attention and Awareness Scale(MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003)

    The MAAS is a 15-item self-report scale designed to

    assess a core characteristic of dispositional

    mindfulness, namely, the general tendency for openor receptive awareness and attention in the present

    moment experience of daily life.

    This measure has demonstrated construct and

    criterion validity in clinical and non-clinical samples

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    12/38

    Measures

    Awareness and Attention Questionnaire II(AAQ-II;Bond et al., In Press)

    The AAQ-II is 10-item self-report scale designed to

    assess psychological flexibility in large population

    based studies.

    The AAQ-II was developed from the AAQ-I resulting

    in enhanced item selection and psychometric

    properties.

    Further psychometric information can be found athttp://www.contextualpsychology.org

    http://www.contextualpsychology.org/http://www.contextualpsychology.org/http://www.contextualpsychology.org/http://www.contextualpsychology.org/
  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    13/38

    Measures

    The Leahy Emotional Schema Scale (LESS; Leahy,2002)

    The LESS is 50-item self-report measure that asks

    participants to report how they have dealt with

    emotional experiences in the last month.

    The scale shows strong psychometric properties and

    has been validated with psychiatric outpatient

    samples (Leahy, 2002).

    It was designed to assess individualsconceptualization of their emotions and utilizes

    fourteen dimensions of a cognitive model of

    emotional processing.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    14/38

    Measures

    Factors/Dimensions of the LESS

    Validation: a belief that other people validate or arereceptive to an individuals emotions.

    Comprehensibility: the perception that an individualsemotions make sense.

    Guilt: the belief that ones emotions are shameful, wrongor embarrassing.

    Simplistic view of emotions: versus complex views andreflects difficulty tolerating ambivalent feelings.

    Relationship to higher values: the belief that anemotional experience can provide insight into values

    clarification. Controllability: the degree to which an individual

    perceives he or she will be in control in the face ofemotion.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    15/38

    Measures

    Factors/Dimensions of the LESS

    Numbness: a lack of strong emotions, emotional isolation,and emotional distancing.

    Rationality: overreliance on logic or anti-emotionality.

    Duration: the prediction of the length of emotionalexperiences.

    Consensus: the belief that others share the same feelings.

    Acceptance: the degree to which an individual allows versusinhibits an emotion.

    Rumination: the tendency to ask unanswerable questionsand dwell on certain emotions.

    Expression: willingness to experience and expressemotions.

    Blame: the tendency to externalize the source of theemotion.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    16/38

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    17/38

    Correlational Analyses:

    MASS & LESS Dimensions

    MASS

    Rationality -.248**

    Duration -.187**

    Consensus .246**

    Acceptance .380**

    Rumination -.279 **

    Expression .228**

    Blame -.190***p< .05, **p< .01

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    18/38

    MASS & LESS Correlations

    MASS positive

    correlations:

    Validation

    Comprehension Higher values

    Control

    Consensus

    Expression

    Acceptance

    MASS negative

    correlations:

    Guilt

    Simplistic views ofemotions

    Numbness

    Rationality

    Duration

    Rumination

    Blame

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    19/38

    Correlational Analyses:

    MASS & AAQ-II

    MASS

    AAQ .493**

    *p< .05, **p< .01

    Dispositional mindfulness as measured by the

    MASS appears to be highly correlated to psychological

    flexibility as measured by the AAQ-II.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    20/38

    Correlational Analyses:

    AAQ-II & LESS Dimensions

    AAQ-II

    Validation .507**

    Comprehension .578**

    Guilt -.642**Simplistic view of emotions -.392**

    Values .274**

    Control .602**

    Numbness -.230**

    *p< .05, **p< .01

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    21/38

    Correlational Analyses:

    AAQ-II & LESS Dimensions

    AAQ-II

    Rationality -.237**

    Duration -.237**

    Consensus .495**

    Acceptance .373**

    Rumination -.497 **

    Expression .163*

    Blame -.364***p< .05, **p< .01

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    22/38

    AAQ-II & LESS Dimensions

    AAQ-II positive

    correlations:

    Validation

    Comprehension Higher values

    Control

    Consensus

    Expression

    Acceptance

    AAQ-II negative

    correlations:

    Guilt

    Simplistic views ofemotions

    Numbness

    Rationality

    Duration

    Rumination

    Blame

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    23/38

    Results

    Our findings suggests that individuals who endorsed a

    higher capacity for mindfulness and those who reported

    a higher degree of psychological flexibility reported:

    Other people will validate or be receptive to their emotions

    Their emotions make sense

    Emotional experiences provide insight into their values

    They can control over their emotions

    Their emotions are normal or shared by others Acceptance of their emotional experiences

    Willingness to experience and express their emotions

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    24/38

    Results

    Our findings suggest that individuals who reported lowerdegrees of dispositional mindfulness and thoseendorsing lower degrees of psychological flexibilityreported:

    Guilt or shame in regards to their emotions Difficulty tolerating ambivalent feelings

    Lacking strong emotional experiences

    Engaging in emotional distancing

    Rationalization or dwelling on their emotions Their emotions last for a long time

    Externalizing the source of their emotional experiences

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    25/38

    Results

    The results of this study also revealed a positive

    correlation between psychological flexibility as

    measured by the AAQ-II and dispositional

    mindfulness as measured by the MASS.

    This indicates that individuals who endorsed a high

    degree of dispositional mindfulness also reported:

    More psychological flexibility

    Less experiential avoidance

    More acceptance of their experiences

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    26/38

    Stepwise Multiple Regression

    Factors of the LESS that represent emotion

    regulation strategies: Rational, Expression,

    Rumination, and Acceptance

    Dependent Variable - Psychological Flexibility(AAQ- II)

    Independent Variables - Dispositional Mindfulness

    (MAAS) and Emotional Schema (LESS) dimensions

    related directly to emotion regulation strategies

    (Rumination, Expression, Rational, Acceptance of

    Feelings)

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    27/38

    Results: Stepwise Multiple

    Regression

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    28/38

    Results: Stepwise Multiple

    Regression

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    29/38

    Results: Stepwise Multiple

    Regression The LESS factors Expression, and Rationalwere not

    significant predictors in this analysis and not included in the

    model.

    A model including the LESS factors Rumination and

    Acceptance of Feelings, as well as dispositional mindfulness,

    as measured by the MAAS accounted for a significant

    proportion of the variance inpsychological flexibility as

    compared to the other LESS factors hypothesized to be

    involved in emotion regulation strategies.

    Rumination was included in the first step of this model,

    followed by Acceptance of Feelings and then by

    dispositional mindfulness. The addition of each variable

    resulted in statistically significant change.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    30/38

    Results: Stepwise Multiple

    Regression These results suggest that emotion regulation

    strategies involving letting go of a ruminative

    thinking style, accepting and allowing emotions as

    they arrive, and an active attending to the present

    moment, interact significantly with psychologicalflexibility.

    The degree to which a person is or is not overly-

    rational, or verbally expressive appears to be less

    important in regards to psychological flexibility.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    31/38

    Discussion

    The correlational results of this study imply a strong

    relationship between dispositional mindfulness,

    psychological flexibility and emotional schemas in adults

    seeking outpatient therapy.

    We also found some evidence for the relationships

    between mindfulness, emotional schema dimensions

    related to emotion regulation strategies and

    psychological flexibility.

    Thus, it may be possible that dispositional mindfulness

    and certain emotional schemas are interacting

    processes involved in the establishment and

    maintenance of psychological flexibility and adaptive

    functioning.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    32/38

    Discussion A possible function of more adaptive emotional

    schemas is a greater degree of psychological

    flexibility and a greater receptive attention to and

    awareness of present experiences.

    However, as correlations do not establish causality

    or direction, it is also possible that, by providing an

    accepting and unbiased emotional experience,

    dispositional mindfulness and psychological

    flexibility allow individuals to perceive the

    uniqueness of a current emotion and respond in a

    flexible and adaptive manner

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    33/38

    Discussion One possible interpretation of the results of the

    stepwise multiple regression would suggest that the

    foundation of psychological flexibility is informed by

    emotion regulation strategies that involve:

    Letting go of a ruminative thinking style

    Acceptance and openness towards emotions

    Active, non-evaluative attending to the present

    moment

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    34/38

    DiscussionThis interpretationwould support thehypothesis inherent inthe ACT hexaflex

    model, thatacceptance, defusion,andcontact with thepresent moment arefundamental

    components ofpsychologicalflexibility.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    35/38

    Study Limitations

    Small sample size and clinical population limits

    generalizablilty

    Self-report assessment measures

    No causality can be assumed

    Additional variables maybe responsible for the

    relationships observed in this study

    Therefore, there is a need for continued research

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    36/38

    For copies of this presentation

    Please contact me at:

    [email protected]

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    37/38

    References

    Bishop, S.R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N.D., Carmody, J.C., Segal, Z.V., Abbey, S., Speca,M., Velting, D. & Devnis, G. (2004) Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology:Science and Practice,11, 230-241.

    Bond, F.W., Hayes, S.C., Baer, R.A., Carpenter, K.M., Orcutt, H.K., Waltz, T. & Zettle, R.D. (In press).Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action QuestionnaireII: A revised measureof psychological flexibility and acceptance.

    Brown, K.W. & Ryan, R.M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822-848.

    Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D.H., Brown, T.A., & Hofman, S.G. (2006). Effects of suppression and acceptanceon emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behavior Research andTherapy, 44,125-1263.

    Chapman, A.L., Gratz, K.L., & Brown, M.Z. (2006). Solving the puzzle of deliberate self harm: The experientialavoidance model. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 371-394.

    Cochrane, A., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holms, Y., Stewart, I., & Luciano, C., (2007). Experiential avoidanceand aversive visual images: Response delays and event-related potentials on a simple task. BehavioralResearch and Therapy, 45,1379-1388.

    Frewen, P.A., Evans, E.M., Maraj, N., Dozois, D.J.A., & Partridge, K. (2008). Letting go: Mindfulness andnegative automatic thinking. Cognitive Therapy Research, 32, 758-774.

    Germer, C.K. (2005). Mindfulness: What is it? Does it matter? In C.K. Germer, R.D. Seigel, & P.R. Fulton (Eds.)Mindfulness and Psychotherapy(pp.3-27) New York: Guilford Press.

  • 8/13/2019 Silberstein Presentation

    38/38

    References

    Hayes, S.C. (2001). Psychology of acceptance and change. In N.J. Smelser & P.B. Baltes (Eds.)International encyclopedia of social & behavioral sciences(pp 27-30).Oxford: Elsevier ScienceLtd.

    Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J., Bond, F., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and CommitmentTherapy: Model, processes, and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 1-25.

    Hayes, S.C., Strosahl K.D. & Wilson, K.G., (1999).Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An

    experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford.

    Hayes, S.C., Wilson, K.G., Gifford, E.V., Follette, V.M., & Strosahl, K., (1996). Experiential avoidanceand behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journalof Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64,1152-1168.

    Izard, C.E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emergingissues.Annual Review of Psychology, 60,1-25.

    Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress,pain, and illness. New York: Bantam Dell.

    Leahy, R. L. (2002). A model of emotional schemas. Cognitive & Behavioral Practice, 9,177-190.

    Leahy, R.L. (2007). Emotion and Psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 14,353-357.