Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    1/14

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    2/14

    Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537

    Visitors’ experience, mood and satisfaction in a heritage context:

    Evidence from an interpretation center

    Carmen de Rojas, Carmen Camarero

    Department of Business and Marketing, University of Valladolid, Avenida Valle de Esgueva, 6, 47011 Valladolid, Spain

    Received 18 June 2006; accepted 8 June 2007

    Abstract

    The study examines how expectations, experiences, and satisfaction are related in the context of cultural tourism and the services

    provided by cultural organizations. A model is proposed that combines two complementary approaches in the analysis of satisfaction: a

    cognitive approach based on quality and disconfirmation and an affective approach based on emotions. The empirical analysis carried

    out on a sample of visitors to an interpretation center allows us to confirm that the perceived quality is a direct determinant of 

    satisfaction, as are emotions. The results also reveal that there is a significant relationship between quality and emotion. Finally, the way

    in which mood state moderates the cognitive path is studied, as generator of visitor satisfaction.

    r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Keywords:  Satisfaction; Expectations; Disconfirmation; Quality; Emotions

    1. Introduction

    The application of marketing in heritage, cultural

    tourism and cultural services is becoming more and more

    crucial. An example is the interest indicated by many

    cultural organizations, such as museums, exhibitions or art

    collections in visitors’ opinions and experience evaluation

    and perceptions’ link to satisfaction. In fact, customer

    experience becomes a key concept in artistic and cultural

    heritage marketing as tourist satisfaction is often deter-

    mined by the global experience obtained. Visitors seek a

    total experience, including leisure, culture, education, and

    social interaction. For this reason, business-oriented

    museums and heritage organizations are increasinglyemphasizing the participation of the public in their policies

    and programs (Gilmore   &  Rentschler, 2002), specifically

    those individuals not usually attracted to traditional

    museums. To create positive experiences for visitors,

    museums organize an increasing number of large-scale

    events and provide a variety of learning experiences. The

    visiting experience becomes more than simply an inspection

    of exhibits, but an opportunity for further elaboration of visitors’ understanding (Colbert, 2003). In this sense, it is

    necessary to note that museums provide more than just

    exhibition. The museum service, i.e., the global product,

    comprises of the exhibition and the other tangible or

    intangible services. These services include the organization

    of courses and seminars, bookshops, restaurants and cafe ´ s,

    brochures and other facilities for better accessibility and

    interpretation, and even the attitudes and values trans-

    mitted to the visitor.

    Consumer satisfaction has been widely debated in

    marketing literature (Bowen, 2001;   Oliver, 1980, 1993;

    Yuksel   &   Yuksel, 2001), even though there is no clearconsensus as to what the determinant variables are. While

    past literature has concentrated on describing satisfaction

    by the evaluation consumers make of perceived quality

    (confirmation/disconfirmation theories) from their expec-

    tations, more recent trends have perceived the emotions

    consumers experienced as the determinant factors in

    creating satisfaction.

    Within this theoretical framework, this study is con-

    cerned with an analysis of the explanatory factors of 

    visitor satisfaction in heritage and cultural expositions

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    www.elsevier.com/locate/tourman

    0261-5177/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2007.06.004

    Corresponding author. Tel.: +34 98342 33 32; fax: +34 98342 38 99.

    E-mail addresses:  [email protected] (C. de Rojas),

    [email protected] (C. Camarero).

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://www.elsevier.com/locate/tourmanhttp://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_5/dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2007.06.004mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_5/dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2007.06.004http://www.elsevier.com/locate/tourmanhttp://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    3/14

    (as promoters of cultural and patrimonial goods) and the

    integration of disparate theoretical approaches. Even if the

    relationship between perceived quality and expectations

    appear essential to evaluate visitor satisfaction, emotions

    and experiences are fundamental to cultural activities. In

    the range of services provided by museums, the inclusion of 

    emotions in the concept of satisfaction is particularlyrelevant given that the majority of services are based upon

    consumers’ participation and experiences (Szymanski   &

    Henard, 2001; Wirtz, Mattila, & Tan, 2000). In the study of 

    museum visitors’ satisfaction, previous works have empha-

    sized the effect of service quality on satisfaction (Caldwell,

    2002;   De Ruyter, Wetzels, Lemmink,   &  Mattsson, 1997;

    Harrison   &   Shaw, 2004), but do not consider emotion.

    There has been little research on the experience of 

    museums’ visitors (Rowley, 1999), and the effect of this

    experience on satisfaction. For instance,   Goulding (2000)

    observes the behavior of museum visitors in order to

    evaluate the impact of exhibitions and experience on visitor

    satisfaction.

    In this research, a model is proposed to explain the

    formation of visitor satisfaction from the relationship

    between their evaluation or cognitive opinion ( perceived

    quality/disconfirmation) and visitor evaluation or affective

    opinion ( positive emotions). Homburg, Koschate, and Hoyer

    (2006)   indicate that few studies have investigated cognitive

    and affective antecedents of customer satisfaction simulta-

    neously. Furthermore, we introduce the moderator ‘visitor’s

    mood’ in order to evaluate how the visitor’s state of mind

    enhances or lessens the effect of their experience on

    satisfaction. Analyzing ‘‘post-purchase’’ visitor behavior com-

    pletes the model. All proposed relationships are tested jointlyby a structural equation model. Empirical analysis performed

    in the Queen Isabel Interpretation Center allows us to make

    conclusions and determine managerial implications.

    2. Cognitive and affective determinants of visitor

    satisfaction

    On a theoretical level, visitor satisfaction is widely

    debated in literature. Satisfaction has been defined

    repeatedly (Oliver, 1997;   Vanhamme, 2000) in forms that

    diverge significantly from one another (Babin   &   Griffin,

    1998;  Szymanski  & Henard, 2001). This suggests that the

    nature of satisfaction is ambiguous. Traditionally satisfac-

    tion was considered to be (i) a cognitive state, (ii) influ-

    enced by previous cognition, and (iii) has relative character

    (the result of the comparison between a subjective

    experience and a previous base of reference) (Bearden   &

    Teel, 1983;   Churchill   &   Surprenant, 1982;   Oliver, 1980;

    Oliver   &   Desarbo, 1988). Recently, however, there has

    been an increasing recognition amongst researchers of 

    satisfaction that a purely cognitive approach may be

    inadequate in modeling satisfaction evaluations. The need

    to understand satisfaction from a more affective perspec-

    tive has been underlined, although always in connection

    with cognitive influence (Oliver, Rust,   &   Varki, 1997;

    Phillips & Baumgartner, 2002; Wirtz & Bateson, 1999). The

    inclusion of affect into the conceptualization of consumer

    satisfaction is particularly important with services, due to

    their experiential nature (Wirtz et al., 2000). In most recent

    definitions, this dual character of satisfaction has been

    recognized (i.e. both cognitive and affective character), as

    well as its relative nature (the comparison between asubjective experience and a previous base of reference). For

    the purposes of this research we will consider that

    satisfaction is reached when visitors attain or transcend

    their relative expectations. Satisfaction is the sensations or

    feelings generated both by cognitive and emotional aspects

    of the goods and services, as well as an accumulated

    evaluation of various components and features.

    Regarding theories and explanatory models of satisfac-

    tion, there are, as already noted, two approaches: cognitive

    and emotional. In following a  cognitive approach to explain

    satisfaction formation, the model most widely recognized is

    the disconfirmation model of expectations (Churchill   &

    Surprenant, 1982;   Oliver, 1980;   Oliver   &   Swan, 1989;

    Prakash, 1984), whereby satisfaction is a function of 

    disconfirmation and disconfirmation is a function of 

    expectations and of fulfillment (Oliver, 1997). The con-

    firmation/disconfirmation theory predicts that satisfaction

    is reached when expectations are met, and that negative

    disconfirmation of expectations will cause dissatisfaction

    while positive disconfirmation will increase satisfaction.

    After a long period of assuming that consumer decisions

    were based on the product quality, use, and benefits, in the

    last two decades market research has begun to study the

    emotions evoked by the stimuli of marketing (Laros   &

    Steenkamp, 2005) from an emotional approach. Despite thisemerging body of research, the study of emotions (theory

    of emotions) in consumer behavior has encountered

    obstacles because of emotions’ ambiguity in structure and

    content (Bagozzi, Gopinath,   &   Nyer, 1999). Regarding

    structure, some researchers examine all emotions from the

    same level of generality, while others specify a hierarchy in

    which specific emotions exemplify other basic emotions

    below the general level (Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson,   &

    O’Connor, 1987;   Storm   &   Storm, 1987). Secondly, we

    encounter a debate concerning the content of emotions.

    Russell and Pratt (1980)   defend the existence of two

    independent dimensions within the emotions: pleasure and

    arousal.

    In an attempt to combine these two theoretical

    approaches (the cognitive and the emotional), we propose

    two complementary paths to develop visitor satisfaction

    with cultural expositions. The cognitive path consists of an

    evaluation of the exhibition quality, a comparison between

    perceived quality and expectations, and a confirmation/

    disconfirmation process that determines the degree of 

    satisfaction achieved. On the other hand, the affective or

    emotional path begins when cultural exhibitions reach and

    even exceed expectations. This provokes pleasure which

    directly influences satisfaction. In the following sections

    this model is developed.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537 526

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    4/14

     2.1. The cognitive approach: expectations, perceived quality,

    and disconfirmation

    Service quality, from the perspective of  Gro ¨ nroos (2000),

    is defined from two dimensions: technical quality (what is

    delivered to the consumer) and the functional service

    quality (how it is delivered). In contrast, Brady and Cronin(2001)   state that the perception of service quality is

    determined by three dimensions: outcome quality, interac-

    tion quality, and physical environment quality. Outcome

    quality is what the customer obtains when the productive

    process ends; interaction quality refers to the interaction

    that takes place while the service is being delivered; and

    environment quality refers to the ambient conditions where

    the service is delivered or the product is sold.

    Oliver (1997)  states that each interpretation consumers

    make are related to different types of expectations.

    Expectations can be defined as previous predictions or

    beliefs that the consumer makes about the results or the

    performance of the product in the future (Higgs, Polonsky,

    &   Hollick, 2005;  Olson   &   Dover, 1979;   Woodruff et al.,

    1983). These expectations are formed using several sources

    of information: advertising and commercial communica-

    tion, word of mouth referrals, or prior experiences.

    Although visitors cannot realistically form expectations

    about a service of which they have little knowledge,

    empirical research suggests that even customers without

    any prior experience do form expectations (McGill   &

    Iacobucci, 1992). According to Oliver (1997), the quality of 

    service is based on perceptions of excellence, so it is logical

    to believe that perceived quality is influenced by the

    expectations of the consumer. Consumers use theirexpectations to evaluate performance. Past research has

    identified the effect of expectations on perceived perfor-

    mance, in that the perceptions are assimilated toward

    expectations (Churchill   &   Surprenant, 1982;   Mattila,

    1998;   Olson   &   Dover, 1979;   Spreng   &   Page, 2001).

    Also  Boulding, Kalra, Staelin, and Zeithaml (1993) argue

    that the expectations are a precursor to perceptions and

    that they are dynamic. Therefore, we propose the first

    hypothesis:

    H1.   Visitors’ expectations positively influence perceived

    quality.

    Perceived quality is considered to be the global judgment

    made by the consumer, by estimating the excellence of a

    service. Therefore, satisfaction and perceived quality will

    be highly interrelated. The majority of market research

    accepts a theoretical framework in which quality leads to

    satisfaction (Dabholkar, Shepherd,   &   Thorpe, 2000;

    Oliver, 1997;  Olsen, 2002), which in turn influences post-

    purchase behavior (De Ruyter, Wetzels,  & Bloemer, 1998;

    Oliver, 1999). It has been empirically confirmed that

    satisfaction is preceded by perceived quality, especially

    when quality is formulated as a specific evaluative belief 

    and satisfaction as a more general evaluation (Olsen, 2002).

    This quality–satisfaction relationship follows the same

    reasoning as classical models of attitude, which suggest

    that attitudes are preceded by beliefs (Fishbein  &  Ajzen,

    1975). Also, in the field of tourist marketing, there are

    authors (Appiah-Adu, Fyall,   &   Singh, 2000;   Bigne ´ ,

    Sa ´ nchez,   &   Sa ´ nchez, 2001) who suggest that perceived

    quality occurs prior to satisfaction. On this basis we

    propose the second hypothesis:

    H2.   Perceived quality positively influences visitor satisfaction.

    Disconfirmation exists when service is inferior or super-

    ior to that which the consumer expected when he/she made

    the decision to buy (Oliver, 1997), thus creating negative or

    positive implications for the experience of service and its

    evaluation. Consumers choose services with the hope that

    what they choose will offer a range of benefits. According

    to the expectation–disconfirmation paradigm (LaBarbera

    &   Mazursky, 1983;   Oliver, 1980;   Spreng, Mackenzie,   &

    Olshavsky, 1996; Tse & Wilton, 1988), consumers calculate

    satisfaction of a product by comparing prior expectations

    against the perceived performance. If the performance is

    superior (inferior) to expectations, a positive (negative)

    disconfirmation is produced and an increase (decrease) in

    satisfaction will be expected. Also, consumer satisfaction is

    a function of expectations and disconfirmation, and prior

    expectations are used as the comparable standard.

    Past empirical research has shown that expectations have

    a significant effect on disconfirmation (Cadotte, Woodruff,

    & Jenkins, 1987; Churchill &  Surprenant, 1982; Patterson,

    1993;   Spreng et al., 1996;   Tse   &   Wilton, 1988). This

    relationship is negative. Therefore it is more likely thathigh expectations are not confirmed (negative disconfirma-

    tion), and that low expectations are. Also, a direct causal

    relationship has been empirically confirmed between

    disconfirmation and satisfaction (Bowen, 2001;   Cadotte

    et al., 1987;   Szymanski   &   Henard, 2001). While some

    authors consider disconfirmation to be the closest ante-

    cedent to satisfaction, and most research illustrates a

    significant effect of disconfirmation on satisfaction (Pat-

    terson, 1993), there are also studies which indicate that this

    relationship is not so clear (Churchill &  Surprenant, 1982;

    Spreng   &   Page, 2001). A possible explanation for this

    discrepancy is the existence of moderator variables, a

    factor we will consider later. Other authors argue that the

    process is more complicated, including   Churchill and

    Surprenant (1982)   who deem that perception affects

    satisfaction in two ways: by disconfirmation and by direct

    experience (quality). According to this, we formulate the

    following hypotheses:

    H3.  Visitor expectations and perceived quality determine

    the level of (dis)confirmation.

    H4.  The level of (dis)confirmation determines the level of 

    visitor satisfaction.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537    527

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    5/14

     2.2. The emotional approach: emotions and pleasure

    According to our discussion, satisfaction research has

    recognized the need to incorporate emotional and affective

    components in the model of consumer satisfaction

    (Liljander &  Strandvik, 1997;  Oliver et al., 1997;  Wirtz &

    Bateson, 1999; Wirtz et al., 2000). More specifically (and asa single example), in the sphere of tourist experiences there

    is a clear need to integrate cognitive and emotional

    concepts to explain satisfaction intentions and behavior

    (Zins, 2002).

    Emotions consist of two independent dimensions:

    pleasure and arousal (Russell   &   Pratt, 1980). Pleasure

    refers to the level at which a person feels well, happy or

    content in a situation, while arousal refers to the extent to

    which a person feels stimulated and active. In the area of 

    marketing applied to cultural tourism and cultural

    organizations, concretely to museums and art expositions,

    we understand that arousal results to be of little relevance

    as stimulating visitors’ activities do not form part of the

    objectives of the organization. Rather, the objectives are

    fundamentally awakening visitors’ interest and increasing

    his/her knowledge about a specific matter so that the visitor

    will experience pleasure.

    Several authors highlight the existing relationship

    between the pleasure felt during the consumer experience

    and the satisfaction obtained. The more pleasure subjects

    experience during the visit, the higher their satisfaction

    (Mano   &   Oliver, 1993;   Oliver, 1993;   Westbrook, 1987;

    Westbrook & Oliver, 1991; Wirtz & Bateson, 1999).

    Previous studies have also tested the relationship

    between disconfirmation and emotions (Menon   &   Dube ´ ,2000;   Oliver et al., 1997;   Wirtz   &   Bateson, 1999),

    corroborating the positive relationship between disconfir-

    mation and the intensity of emotion in order to explain

    consumer satisfaction (Woodruff et al., 1983). However,

    the path between disconfirmation and pleasure has not

    always been confirmed.  Bigne ´ , Andreu, and Gnoth (2005)

    do not find a significant relationship between disconfirma-

    tion and pleasure in a tourist context. In the context of 

    museums and exhibitions, the expectations are more ideal

    than predictive.   Higgs et al. (2005)   also note that art

    museums and art exhibitions attract large numbers of 

    novice patrons who are without prior experience but hold

    ideal expectations, which are altered during their first (and

    perhaps only) visit. Therefore, disconfirmation is more

    determined by the perceived quality than by expectations.

    In this situation it is the perceived quality and not the

    disconfirmation of expectations that is the real determinant

    of visitors’ emotions and pleasure. Therefore, we hypothe-

    size a positive relationship between the cognitive experience

    (the perceived quality), and the emotional experience (the

    intensity of emotions and pleasure). A shortfall in service

    performance can cause displeasure, whereas a high

    performance can cause pleasure. In this sense,   Chebat

    and Michon (2003)   identify a significant relationship

    between product quality and pleasure. Then,

    H5.   Perceived quality positively influences the pleasure

    dimension of emotions.

    H6.  The pleasure dimension of emotions positively influ-

    ences visitor satisfaction.

     2.3. Consequences of satisfaction in cultural tourism:

    intensification

    Research on satisfaction and services suggests that

    satisfaction is an important antecedent to the post-

    purchase attitude and the intentions to repeat the purchase,

    as well as in many other cases of good intentions for

    behavior (Anderson, 1994;   Anderson   &   Sullivan, 1993;

    Cronin   &   Taylor, 1992;   Fornell, 1992;   Keaveney, 1995;

    Oliver, 1980;   Oliver   &   Swan, 1989;   White   &   Yu, 2005;

    Zeithaml, Berry,   &   Parasuraman, 1996). In this sense, it

    has been suggested that tourist satisfaction affects the

    intensification of the visit, this being understood as an

    interest or motivation on the part of the visitor. Moreconcretely in the context of cultural tourism, it may be

    described as the intention to purchase guides, publications,

    and souvenirs and presents (Kim   &   Littrell, 1999).

    Although a variable not widely considered in literature,

    Bigné and Andreu (2004)   propose—although do not

    confirm—a direct influence of satisfaction on the intensi-

    fication experience, measured as visitor interest in buying

    different products and souvenirs related to the location or

    event. It is expected that satisfied visitors will, prolong their

    stay, and take more interest in any of the collected aspects

    within the cultural exposition. This is transformed into

    acquisition, including specific materials relative to the visit

    (renting of audio-guides or commemorative coins), as well

    as miscellaneous souvenirs (key chains, pens). Therefore,

    H7.  The higher the level of visitor satisfaction, the higher

    the level of visitor intensification.

    3. The moderator role of moods

    Moods occur ‘‘when the cognitive system is maintained

    in an emotion mode for a period’’ (Oatley, 1992, p. 64,

    taken from Bagozzi et al., 1999). Moods can be caused by

    the human system (such as sickness, fatigue, previous

    exercise, and good health), or pharmacological agents,

    general conditions of the environment, and side effects of 

    activities (heat, noise, changes in surroundings, stress)

    (Frijda, 1986).

    According to Bagozzi et al. (1999), the line between an

    emotion and mood is frequently difficult to draw. They

    propose that mood is a longer lasting state (from a few

    hours to days), and lower in intensity than an emotion.

    A secondary difference is that emotion is intentional (it has

    an object or reference object), while mood is unintentional

    and global or diffused (Frijda, 1993). Lastly, moods are not

    as directly connected to intentions of action or explicit

    actions, as are many emotions. Bagozzi et al. (1999) suggest

    that mood can affect the cognitive and affective process of 

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537 528

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    6/14

    the consumer, therefore directly influencing processes such

    as: (a) the recovery of information, i.e., people in a good

    mood have a better memory of material learned while in a

    positive mental state than in a negative or neutral one;

    (b) encoding of information, i.e., happy people spend more

    time examining positive information than negative and,

    therefore, remember it more effectively (Forgas & Bower,1987); (c) the state of learning; when the survey subjects are

    in a good mood the average level of memory can be higher;

    and (d) asymmetric effects of mood, i.e., a good mood

    favors positive memories and inhibits negative. In the same

    way, people who are in a good mood evaluate stimuli more

    positively than those in a bad or neutral mood. Moods do

    not interrupt ongoing thought processes and behaviors, but

    they do color our day-to-day events (Luong, 2005).

    On this basis we can suggest that a better initial mood on

    the part of visitors will have repercussions on their

    reactions to stimuli presented to them and to the emotions

    provoked. Specifically, a good mood will strengthen the

    following relationships: quality–satisfaction, disconfirma-

    tion–satisfaction, and emotion–satisfaction. Consequently,

    H8.   Mood acts as a moderator in the process of formation

    of visitor satisfaction.

    H8a.  Favourable mood strengthens the effect of quality on

    satisfaction.

    H8b.   Favourable mood strengthens the effect of discon-

    firmation on satisfaction.

    H8c.   Favourable mood strengthens the effect of pleasure

    on satisfaction.

    The proposed hypotheses are represented in  Fig. 1.

    4. Methodology

    4.1. Sample and data collection

    In order to test the proposed hypotheses, the empirical

    study performed is based on information collected by means

    of a questionnaire presented to visitors of the cultural centercalled the  Queen Isabel Interpretation Center. The center is

    located in the Royal and Testamentary Palace in Valladolid,

    Spain. The Royal and Testamentary Palace, in which Queen

    Isabel I of Castile (1451–1504) lived, made her will, and

    died, was restored and converted into a historical inter-

    pretation center. In this center visitors discover the life, the

    reign, and the personality of Isabel the Catholic by means of 

    museographic and exhibitory resources. Moreover, two

    rooms are devoted to the Queen’s will.

    We have selected an interpretation center of History for

    one main reason. This museum is one where the quality of 

    resources and museographic materials are essential to

    involve and energize the visitor and to achieve a

    satisfactory experience. Moreover, in this case, the location

    is significant. Valladolid, the chief town of Castile, is where

    Queen Isabel I was born, lived, reigned, and died. It was an

    historical period marked by the influential role of Castile in

    history, such as the birth of Spain as a nation, and the

    discovery of America. This context creates a feeling of 

    affection in local visitors; therefore, emotions can be

    aroused, and have an important influence on overall

    satisfaction.

    The choice of a single center for the collection of data

    was an attempt to place the research in a similar context for

    all visitors, so that any possible influence of contextualfactors (cultural and tourist factors and adjacent services)

    on variables studied (satisfaction, perceived quality, emo-

    tion) could be avoided.

    Before proceeding to the quantitative study, some

    interviews with the museum manager and employees were

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    MOOD

    EXPECTATIONS

    DISCONFIRMATION

    PLEASURE

    SATISFACTION

    INTENSIFICATION

    H6

    H3H1

    H5

    H8a

    H2

    H3

    H8c

    H8b

    H4

    H7

    PERCEIVED

    QUALITY

    Fig. 1. Proposed model.

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537    529

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    7/14

    performed in order to know tourists’ reactions and

    emotions during their visits. They explained that in the

    Queen Isabel I Interpretation Center, the main resource

    of the exhibition is the Royal Palace itself. The building

    is the leading protagonist, given that it was where the

    Queen wrote her last wishes, and died. Therefore the

    Royal Palace has been declared as a historical site of great cultural interest. The whole exhibition and its

    resources are structured around this event; the Palace

    remains the primary tourist attraction, while the inter-

    pretation center is the secondary element of attraction.

    We must point out that this Center organizes guided visits.

    The rooms dedicated to the Queen’s Death and Testament

    are of special importance. The reproductions of her last

    will and the stage design of the bedroom where she died

    attract beyond all doubt. One unexpected aspect is the

    study of the Queen’s physical appearance (blond hair and

    bright eyes compared to the stereotypical Castilian looks).

    Equally surprising is the Palace’s area today (barely

    10% of what it was during the period of its greatest

    splendor). Finally, with regard to the level of implication

    and concentration, the guided tour design facilitates not

    only these aspects, but also the visitor’s interaction at

    certain moments (group dynamics students and theatrical

    re-enactments).

    We developed a questionnaire to test the aforementioned

    hypotheses. The questionnaire was divided into two parts.

    The first part covered the mood and the expectations

    before the visit. The second part referred to the perceived

    quality, the disconfirmation of expectations, the pleasure

    and the satisfaction after the visit. A preliminary draft of 

    the questionnaire was developed and administered to thecenter’s manager and to some employees (who have

    contact with the visitors) and who helped us make minor

    adjustments.

    The first survey was performed during the months of 

    March and April 2005 and the total number of ques-

    tionnaires collected was 180. A second set of data was

    collected during June, July, and August 2006, providing

    104 valid questionnaires. In order to test non-response bias

    we compared the former and the later responses along all

    response items for each of the scales. No significant

    difference between the two groups was found. Every fourth

    visitor who entered the center was surveyed. We employed

    survey takers who were instructed to collect data by

    presenting the questionnaire before the visitor entered the

    attraction. Those surveyed were asked to answer the first

    part of the questionnaire, (i.e. the questions referring to

    their mood and expectations). After answering these

    questions, the visitor held onto the questionnaire during

    the visit, and the second part of the questionnaire was

    completed upon exit. Another was employed to collect the

    questionnaires upon visitors’ exit.

    Concerning the representativeness of the sample, it is

    relevant to note that although we performed a convenience

    sample the total number of visitors in museums of History

    in Spain during 2004 was 3,086,662, 6.5% of total

    visitation to museums and collections (data provided

    by the Spanish Ministry of Culture). In our case, the

    reduced dimensions and the specialization of this center

    are the reasons it can receive only a few visitors per day.

    The number of visitors during 2005 was 14,839, and

    during 2006 it (until July) was 7442. The questionnaire

    was offered to approximately 1600 visitors and 284valid answers were obtained, corresponding to a response

    rate of 17.7%. The final sample consisted of 48% men

    and 52% women. By age groups, 3.2% of respondents

    were under 18, 43.3% between 18 and 35, 47% between 35

    and 60, and 12.4% over 60. By education, 3.2% of the

    sample had none, 10.1% had finished primary school,

    29.5% secondary school, and 57.1% had attended higher

    education. As we have aforementioned, few works have

    analyzed the satisfaction of museums’ visitors and of those

    that did, most adopted a qualitative approach. In any case,

    if we compare the demographic characteristics of our

    sample with those of   Harrison and Shaw (2004), we can

    conclude that they are very similar.

    4.2. Measurement of variables

    The scale of   expectations   is constructed of seven items

    adapted from the scale proposed by   Higgs et al. (2005)

    and also applied to the case of museums and exhibition

    centers. The scale of   perceived quality   (six items) was

    prepared from the proposal of  Brady and Cronin (2001),

    and considers the three dimensions of quality: outcome

    quality, interaction quality, and physical environment

    quality. All of the included items contain a parallel

    with the proposals as a measure of expectations. Theseitems were adapted to reflect the context of a museum.

    Outcome quality was measured as the educational

    and instructive experience and the excellence of the objects

    and materials exposed; interaction quality focused on

    the treatment received and the employees’ willingness to

    look after the visitor; and physical environment quality

    referred to the center’s installations, informative panels and

    atmosphere. In a similar way, expectations were measured

    in terms of service provided by the employees, the quality

    of the installations (panels, lighting, spaces), uniqueness

    of the exhibition and historical interest, and educational

    experience. The variable disconfirmation was measured

    based on an item that reflects the general level of 

    confirmation-disconfirmation. To measure confirmation-

    disconfirmation, a 5 point Likert scale was employed

    in which the question ‘‘in comparison with what in

    general you expected from the visit to the interpretation

    center, it has been’’ was evaluated by the following

    rankings; [1] much worsey [5] much better than expected.

    Pleasure   (six items) was measured based on the scale of 

    Russell and Pratt (1980), and to measure   satisfaction

    (five items) the   Oliver (1997)   scale was used and inclu-

    ded two additional that measured the intention of 

    word-of-mouth recommendation. The scale to measure

    the variable   intensification   (2 items) was prepared by

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537 530

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    8/14

    the authors, adapting it to the objectives of this study.

    Finally, the scale used to measure   mood   was prepared

    based on three indicators (scale of differential semantics) to

    reflect the level of enthusiasm or excitement felt by the

    tourist before beginning the visit. The group of scales

    proposed and the corresponding descriptions are shown in

    Appendix A.Even though some of the scales proposed had already

    been validated by literature, they were all submitted to a

    process of validation by means of an exploratory factorial

    analysis followed by a confirmatory analysis (Lisrel 8.7).

    To validate the scale of quality two items measuring

    interaction quality had to be eliminated because of the

    scarce variability of these items (the normality could not be

    warranted). With the rest of items a confirmatory factor

    analysis was performed. The results indicate that there is

    no divergence between the dimensions outcome quality and

    physical environment quality; therefore they were inte-

    grated into the same dimension. The scales on expectations

    and pleasure were also validated by two confirmatory

    factor analysis and the scales of satisfaction and intensi-

    fication were validated jointly by the reduced number of 

    items. The values of goodness of fit for each scale or group

    of scales and the parameters lambda for each variable are

    shown in Appendix A.

    Once the convergence of the scales was validated the

    values of reliability were calculated by the Cronbach alpha

    and AVE in each case (see Appendix A). In all cases the

    reliability was above the recommended values. To obtain a

    guarantee of discriminate validity, we used the confidence

    intervals. The correlations between latent variables

    and their confidence intervals were calculated and thevalue 1 did not appear in any case between these

    intervals. Correlations are shown in   Table 1. In all cases

    the variance extracted of each variable exceeds the value of 

    its squared correlation with the other variables, which also

     justifies the discriminant validity of the scales (Anderson &

    Gerbing, 1988).

    5. Analysis and results

    The following step of the analysis consisted of the

    evaluation of the proposed model by means of a path

    analysis. First, each of the scales was reduced to onlyone indicator or index (extracted factor for each latent

    variable based on CFA). With these values, and after

    calculating the error measurement of each indicator

    based on the values of reliability, the proposed model

    was evaluated. The result of the evaluation is shown in

    Fig. 2. The indicators of goodness of fit (w2(7) ¼ 19.28,

     p ¼ 0.007; GFI ¼ 0.976; AGFI¼ 0.927; CFI ¼ 0.981;

    RMSEA ¼ 0.082; PNFI¼ 0.453; PGFI¼ 0.325) are

    clearly placed within the recommended limits and areconsidered as proof of good fit.

    In order to analyze the moderating effect of mood, the

    scale created is reduced to one single factor that indicates

    the level of enthusiasm, animation or excitement which the

    visitor finds him or herself in before beginning the visit.

    Following the procedure proposed by   Ping (1995), and

    after centering the variables to attenuate error caused by

    multicolineality (Aiken   &   West, 1991), we proceeded to

    analyze the proposed model again, this time introducing

    the interaction of mood with perceived quality, disconfir-

    mation and pleasure. The result of the evaluation is shown

    in Fig. 3. The indicators of goodness of fit (w2(19) ¼ 80.96,

     p ¼ 0.000; GFI ¼ 0.937; AGFI¼ 0.850; CFI ¼ 0.945;

    RMSEA ¼ 0.111; PNFI ¼ 0.491; PGFI¼ 0.396) are lo-

    cated within the recommended limits and are considered

    proof of a good fit. Parsimony indexes are slightly better

    than in the previous model.

    As can be concluded from these results, hypotheses

    H1 and H2 are confirmed. In the line of previous

    research we observed that expectations determine perceived

    quality in the visit and that perceived quality, in turn,

    is an indicator of satisfaction. Also, regarding H3, we

    confirm that disconfirmation is positively determined

    by perceived quality and negatively influenced by the

    expectations of the visitor. However, the existence of asignificant effect of disconfirmation on satisfaction (H4)

    was not demonstrated, a result already reported by

    some authors (like   Spreng   &   Page, 2001), who consider

    the existence of variables that moderate the relation-

    ship between expectations and satisfaction as possibly

    being the level of confidence visitors have in their

    expectations. In short, it is demonstrated that the cognitive

    approach of satisfaction is validated by the path of 

    perceived quality, but not by the route of dissatisfaction

    of expectations.

    Regarding the emotional approach, we validate the

    positive effect of perceived quality on pleasure (H5), and

    the effect of pleasure on satisfaction (H6), in such a way

    that we can see how emotions are also generators of 

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    Table 1

    Matrix of correlations

    Expectations Perceived quality Disconfirmation Pleasure Satisfaction

    Perceived quality 0.363

    Disconfirmation 0.118 0.581

    Pleasure 0.245 0.543 0.441

    Satisfaction 0.347 0.731 0.542 0.637

    Disconfirmation 0.057 0.248 0.268 0.132 0.265

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537    531

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    9/14

    satisfaction. It seems that the formation of positive

    emotions derives from an evaluation of positive quality.

    This means that the model of beliefs–attitudes is again

    confirmed in the formation of visitor satisfaction. As for

    the relationship between satisfaction and intensification

    of the use or service (H7), we also find a positive

    and significant coefficient to confirm the proposed hypo-thesis, unlike previous studies (Bigne ´   &   Andreu, 2004)

    that failed to demonstrate this effect. Nevertheless, we

    observe that the effect of satisfaction on intention, in

    this case to buy or try other related services, is not

    excessively high.

    Finally, and in relation to hypothesis H8, the interaction

    between mood and disconfirmation of expectations (H8c)

    has been validated, indicating that a more positive mood

    (more enthusiasm, animation or excitement) makes the

    disconfirmation of expectations reinforce the satisfaction of the visitor. However, we have found an unexpected nega-

    tive effect in the interaction between mood and perceived

    quality on satisfaction. The greater is visitors’ enthusiasm

    and excitation before the visit, while the lower is the effect

    of their cognitive experiences (perceived quality) on

    satisfaction. The moderator ‘effect of mood’ on the

    emotions felt (measures of pleasure) (H8b) was not

    significant.

    6. Discussion and conclusions

    This study began with the aim of analyzing a specificaspect of marketing applied to cultural tourism, art and

    cultural exhibitions, and the formation of visitor satisfac-

    tion. Our proposal is based on combining two comple-

    mentary approaches in the analysis of satisfaction: a

    cognitive approach, according to which satisfaction derives

    from stimuli and quality received and from the confirma-

    tion–disconfirmation of expectations; and an affective

    approach, according to which satisfaction also responds

    to emotions that arouse certain stimuli in the visitor and

    moderate visitors’ experiences’ effect on satisfaction. This

    was developed in the context of cultural exhibitions and

    our analysis has contemplated visitors’ expectations andmoods before the visit, and how they influence satisfaction.

    We consider this context, a heritage tourism attraction, to

    be a relevant contribution given that previous works

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    DISCONFIRMATION

    PLEASURE

    INTENSIFICATION

    EXPECTATIONS

    SATISFACTION

    PERCEIVED

    QUALITY

    0.445

    (6.728)-0.227

    (-3.750)

    0.092

    (1.372)0.497

    (6.077)

    0.813

    (13.016)

    0.382

    (6.531)

    0.322

    (4.176)

    0.600

    (9.774)

    Fig. 2. Results of path analysis.

    DISCONFIRMATION

    PLEASURE

    INTENSIFICATION

    PERCEIVED

    QUALITY

    EXPECTATIONS

    SATISFACTION

    Mood ∗ QualityMood ∗

    DisconfirmationMood ∗Pleasure

    0.809

    (13.029)-0.205

    (-3.413)

    0.005

    (0.091)0.488

    (5.768)

    0.919

    (6.808)

    0.477

    (7.808)

    0.246

    (3.195)

    0.685(11.802)

    0.190

    (2.461)0.118

    (1.392)

    -0.261

    (-2.121)

    Fig. 3. Results of path analysis (with moderator effects).

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537 532

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    10/14

    analyzing the impact of expectations, quality, and emo-

    tions on satisfaction have focused on entertainment

    attractions. Tourists do not choose museums and cultural

    exhibitions because of a cursory interest, but because they

    have a familiarity with the exhibition’s content, in our case,

    with Spanish history.

    The first implication of our work is the confirmation of our model due to its high validity and explanatory

    power. Our model of satisfaction is unusual in that it

    combines cognitive and emotional aspects. In fact, it has

    been illustrated that the model knowledge–attitude– 

    behavior is completely valid in the explanation of the path

    followed by the visitor in the evaluation of his/her

    satisfaction levels and subsequent behavior. Visitor’

    expectations are affected by both cognitive (perceived

    quality) and emotional (pleasure) experiences; it is possible

    to view these as two complementary paths that result in

    satisfaction. If we compare these two pathways, we can

    affirm that the effect of perceived quality on satisfaction is

    greater than the effect of emotions. Moreover, pleasure, in

    this cultural context, appears not to be an external feeling

    but closely related to the cognitive experience (perceived

    quality). Cognitive factors’ greater influence can be

    explained by the visitor’s experience. Although this was

    the first visit to the interpretation center for most visitors,

    their profiles indicated that most had attended higher

    education (57.1%). Moreover, the historical theme of the

    interpretation center continues to attract more involved

    and experienced visitors, most of whom have a relatively

    high knowledge of museums and exhibitions.   Homburg

    et al. (2006) state that as experience accumulates the impact

    of affective factors on customer satisfaction decreases andthe impact of cognitive factors increases. Affect has a

    greater impact on the satisfaction judgments in earlier

    stages of the satisfaction development process, when

     judgments require a higher degree of constructive proces-

    sing. Applied to context of cultural exhibitions, visitors are

    likely to have previously gained experience of museums or

    centers and consequently the role of cognition becomes

    more prominent when experience accumulates (Homburg

    et al., 2006).

    However, we have not proved the effect of disconfirma-

    tion on satisfaction. An explanation of this result can be

    found in visitor’s confidence of expectations.   Spreng and

    Page (2001) indicate that the level to which expectation is

    held may influence the degree or extent to which

    expectation is utilized in post-purchase evaluations. When

    a consumer holds an expectation with little confidence it

    will be fulfilled, he/she is not surprised when it does not

    eventuate. Disconfirmation of this expectation will not

    have a strong effect on satisfaction. An expectation formed

    based on information obtained from a highly credible,

    expert source, is likely to be held with more confidence

    than one developed from casual word-of-mouth conversa-

    tions (Spreng &  Page, 2001). In this research, expectations

    have not yet been created by prior experience (as

    aforementioned, in most cases it was the visitors’ inaugural

    visit), or by commercial sources, as the advertisements of 

    this interpretation center are largely brochures. Therefore,

    it is likely that expectations are based on information

    gathered from brochures and word-of-mouth conversa-

    tions, and thus the level of confidence in this information is

    not high.

    Another implication refers to expectations. In theestimated model it is evidenced that expectations have a

    lower effect on disconfirmation than perceived quality.

    Wirtz and Bateson (1999)  indicate that when expectations

    have a low influence on disconfirmation, a situation of no

    disconfirmation and no complex cognitive processes occurs

    at the same time. Experience rather than disconfirmation is

    therefore the determinant of affective states.

    An additional conclusion centers on the implications of 

    visitor satisfaction. We have not examined loyalty (which

    is a regular consequence of satisfaction in marketing

    literature) because of its low relevancy in this context.

    However, we have analyzed the intensification of use by

    measuring the purchase of related products or material,

    and we have found that intensification does appear to

    be a behavior correlated to visitor satisfaction (although

    this correlation is weak). Part of our study is the analysis

    of the role played by the mood of the individual in the

    formation of satisfaction. Our results enable us to state

    that a positive mood reinforces the effects of disconfirma-

    tion of expectations during the process to final satisfaction,

    but does not influence the effect of emotions experienced.

    It seems then that the mood strengthens the cognitive

    path towards satisfaction, but does not influence the

    emotional pathway. A non-expected result is that a

    positive mood reduces the effect of perceived quality onsatisfaction.   Bagozzi et al. (1999)  say that when people

    attribute their mood to a different situation or object than

    that being evaluated, mood’s effect may disappear. We can

    add that an unimpressed and calm visitor is more

    predisposed to base satisfaction on the perception of 

    quality.

    Regarding   managerial implications, the study suggests

    the importance of visitor experience in cognitive and

    emotional aspects as bases for satisfaction. Although

    cognitive and affective dimensions are central to managers,

    one does not go without the other (Chebat   &   Michon,

    2003). This seems to be in line with the new strategies and

    attitudes adopted by managers of museums and cultural

    centers at the time of designing and planning their

    businesses. The presentation of the cultural product

    (location, internal distribution, walkways, lighting or

    informative panels) contributes to stimulate the interest

    and the involvement of the visitor. Also, the interpretation

    and the intangibles which surround the cultural product

    (contextualization, personalization, creation of a dialogue,

    and interaction with the visitor in order to create an

    experience, feelings, and sensations) allows the visitor to

    understand, feel, and relive the heritage. If the visitor is

    offered an adequate interpretation of that being contem-

    plated, it increases his/her involvement and increases the

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537    533

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    11/14

    possibility that the visitor will spend more time, stop at a

    gift shop, encourage other visits, and even help in

    preservation of the heritage. Management in tourism may

    also learn the importance of offering convenient cognitive

    and emotional experiences. When a destination is based on

    the offer of museums and heritage exhibitions or events, it

    should be analyzed whether or not tourists may needvarious stimuli gain a positive experience. For instance, the

    satisfaction of a more involved tourist could be more

    related to the extent to which the exhibition is unique and

    original, while for less-involved tourists satisfaction could

    be determined by emotions which arise from stimuli other

    than the exhibition itself.

    Evidently, the work presented is not without  limitations.

    Firstly, we have introduced the study of a concrete case.

    The application of the study to other cultural sectors would

    allow for wider generalizations to be made from the results

    obtained. For instance, in a case study the impact of the

    type or novelty of exhibitions on visitors’ expectations,

    pleasure and satisfaction cannot be appreciated. For future

    research, we leave scope for the inclusion of other factors

    such as the confidence of consumers in their expectations

    or context in which the service is being placed. Confidence

    and context could be variables that moderate the effect of 

    expectations, quality or emotions on satisfaction. This

    would require us to analyze the model using a larger

    sample of a wide range of museums, exhibitions and

    cultural centers. Another possibility is the analysis of the

    moderating effect of personality in the process of generat-

    ing satisfaction via emotional or cognitive paths. For

    instance,   Gountas and Gountas (2007)   propose the

    influence of consumers’ personality orientations on positive

    and negative emotional states during the service interaction

    and on the overall service satisfaction. Regarding this

    context, we have not differentiated between the segments of 

    visitors. It would be interesting to compare the expecta-tions and evaluations of tourists to those of locals and

    more-involved visitors. We also consider the application of 

    this model to be interesting, with its possible variations, to

    the case of artistic products (theater, performances,

    concerts) where the role of emotions could prove to be

    more relevant in the generation of satisfaction than the role

    of perceived quality, or the disconfirmation of expectations

    due to the increased evaluation difficulty for spectators. We

    should note that as the study has not considered interaction

    perceived quality, we have not proved the impact of service

    personnel behavior on visitor affect and pleasure.

    Appendix A

    Variables and items (see Table A1).

    Appendix B

    See Table B1.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    Table A1

    Variable Item Description Mean SD   l a   AVE

    Expectations EXP1 I think that the center will have professionals

    disposed to offer me information and respond to my

    needs

    4.14 0.94 0.86 0.892 0.652

    EXP2 I expect to receive good treatment from the

    employees

    4.30 0.84 0.85

    EXP3 In this center I believe I will find modern,

    technologically advanced installations

    3.86 0.95 0.69

    EXP4 I believe that the center will have informative panels

    and adequate lighting and use of spaces

    4.03 0.94 0.88

    EXP5 I believe that the content of the exhibition will be

    unique and different from that of other centers

    3.64 1.01 0.73

    EXP6 I believe that this center is going to offer me an

    interesting educational or instructive experience

    3.94 0.97 –  b

    EXP7 I expect that the objects, material, and information

    exhibited to be of high cultural and historical interest

    4.06 0.94 0.81

    w2(7) ¼ 21.395 (P ¼ 0.003); GFI ¼ 0.973; AGFI ¼ 0.920; CFI ¼ 0.991; RMSEA ¼ 0.08

    Perceived quality QUA1 The treatment received by the center employees has

    been excellent (interaction quality)

    4.65 0.57 –  b 0.865 0.650

    QUA2 At all times the center employees have demonstrated

    their willingness to look after me correctly

    (interaction quality)

    4.65 0.63 –  b

    QUA3 The installations in the center are better than those in

    other centers or museums I have visited (physical

    environment quality)

    3.29 0.98 0.85

    QUA4 3.82 0.98 0.82

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537 534

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    12/14

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    Table A1 (continued )

    Variable Item Description Mean SD   l a   AVE

    I evaluate the informative panels positively and the

    atmosphere created in the center (physical

    environment quality)

    QUA5 I consider the visit to the center to have been a good

    educational or instructive experience (outcome

    quality)

    3.98 0.99 0.75

    QUA6 I consider the exhibition of the objects and materials

    in the center to be excellent (outcome quality)

    3.49 1.11 0.80

    Disconfirmationa DIS1 In comparison with what you expected from the visit

    in general to the center of interpretation, it has been:

    much worse/worse/the same/better/much better than

    expected

    3.57 0.86 0.97

    w2(5) ¼ 14.789 (P ¼ 0.011); GFI ¼ 0.978; AGFI ¼ 0.935; CFI ¼ 0.989; RMSEA ¼ 0.08

    Pleasurea PLE1 Angry/content 4.10 0.83 0.89 0.900 0.699

    PLE2 Bored/entertained 3.92 0.97 0.82

    PLE3 Unhappy/happy 3.93 0.90 0.92

    PLE4 Disappointed/impressed 3.64 0.98 0.76

    PLE5 Unhappy/joyful 3.91 0.94 0.78

    PLE6 Unhappy/delighted 3.12 1.17 –  b

    w2(4) ¼ 9.552 (P ¼ 0.048); GFI ¼ 0.985; AGFI ¼ 0.946; CFI ¼ 0.995; RMSEA ¼ 0.07

    Satisfaction SAT1 This is one of the best interpretation centers I could

    have visited

    3.28 1.01 –  b 0.903 0.751

    SAT2 I am pleased with my decision to visit this

    interpretation center

    3.98 0.87 0.87

    SAT3 I have really had a good time, I have had fun in this

    center

    3.78 0.95 0.81

    SAT4 I will recommend someone to visit this center 3.93 1.03 0.95

    SAT5 I will say positive things about this interpretation

    center

    3.86 1.01 0.83

    Intensification INT1 I have bought a book or guide for more information 1.97 1.37 0.78 0.700 0.640

    INT2 If there were a shop in this center I would buy a

    souvenir

    2.80 1.46 0.82

    w2(8) ¼ 18.354 (P ¼ 0.018); GFI ¼ 0.975; AGFI ¼ 0.935; CFI ¼ 0.989; RMSEA ¼ 0.07

    Mooda MOO1 Unimpressed/enthused 2.89 1.09

    MOO2 Calm/excited 2.83 1.05

    MOO3 Relaxed/animated 2.90 1.19

    aScale of differential semantic.bEliminated items.

    Table B1

    Questionnaire

    Variable Question

    Expectationsa I think that the center will have professional available to offer me information and respond to my needs

    I expect to receive good treatment from the employees

    In this center I believe I will find modern, technologically advanced installationsI believe that the center will have informative panels and adequate lighting and use of spaces

    I believe that the content of the exhibition will be unique and different from that of other centers

    I believe that this center is going to offer me an interesting educational or instructive experience

    I expect that the objects, material and information exhibited to be of high cultural and historical interest

    Perceived qualitya The treatment received by the center employees has been excellent (interaction quality)

    At all times the center employees have demonstrated their willingness to look after me correctly (interaction quality)

    The installations in the center are better than those in other centers or museums I have visited (physical environment

    quality)

    I evaluate the informative panels positively and the atmosphere created in the center (physical environment quality)

    I consider the visit to the center to have been a good educational or instructive experience (outcome quality)

    I consider the exhibition of the objects and materials in the center to be excellent (outcome quality)

    Disconfirmationb In comparison with what you expected from the visit in general to the center of interpretation, it has been: much worse/

    worse/the same/better/much better than expected

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537    535

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    13/14

    References

    Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991).   Multiple regression: Testing and 

    interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Anderson, E. W. (1994). Cross-category variation in customer satisfaction

    and retention.  Marketing Letters,  5(1), 19–30.

    Anderson, E. W., & Sullivan, M. W. (1993). The antecedents and

    consequences of customer satisfaction for firms. Marketing Science, 12,

    125–143.

    Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling

    in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach.   Psycho-

    logical Bulletin,  103, 411–423.

    Appiah-Adu, K., Fyall, A., & Singh, S. (2000). Marketing culture and

    customers retention in the tourism industry.   The Service Industries

    Journal ,  20(2), 95–113.

    Babin, B. J., & Griffin, M. (1998). The nature of satisfaction. An

    updated examination and analysis.  Journal of Business Research,  4(2),

    127–136.

    Bagozzi, R. P., Gopinath, M., & Nyer, P. (1999). The role of emotions in

    marketing.  Journal Academy of Marketing Science,  27 (2), 184–206.

    Bearden, W. O., & Teel, J. E. (1983). Selected determinants of consumer

    satisfaction and complaint reports.   Journal of Marketing Research,

     20(1), 21–28.

    Bigne ´ , J. E., & Andreu, L. (2004). Modelo cognitivo-afectivo de la

    satisfaccio ´ n en servicios de ocio y turismo.  Cuadernos de Economı́  a y

    Direcció n de la Empresa,  21, 89–120.Bigne ´ , J. E., Andreu, L., & Gnoth, J. (2005). The theme park experience:

    An analysis of pleasure, arousal and satisfaction.   Tourism Manage-

    ment,  26 (6), 833–844.

    Bigne ´ , J. E., Sa ´ nchez, M. I., & Sa ´ nchez, J. (2001). Tourism image,

    evaluation variables and after purchase behaviour inter-relationship.

    Tourism Management,  22(6), 607–616.

    Boulding, W., Kalra, A., Staelin, R., & Zeithaml, V. (1993). A dynamic

    process of service quality: From expectations to behavioural inten-

    tions. Journal of Marketing Research,  30(1), 7–27.

    Bowen, D. (2001). Antecedents of consumer satisfaction and dissatisfac-

    tion on long haul inclusive tours: A reality check on theoretical

    considerations.   Tourism Management,  22(1), 49–61.

    Brady, M., & Cronin, J. (2001). Some new thoughts on conceptualizing

    perceived service quality: A hierarchical approach.  Journal of Market-

    ing,  65(3), 34–49.

    Cadotte, E. R., Woodruff, R. B., & Jenkins, R. L. (1987). Expectations

    and norms in models of consumer satisfaction.  Journal of Marketing

    Research,  24(3), 305–314.

    Caldwell, N. (2002). (Rethinking) the measurement of service quality in

    museums and galleries.   International Journal of Nonprofit and 

    Voluntary Sector Marketing,  7 (2), 161–171.

    Chebat, J. C., & Michon, R. (2003). Impact of ambient odors on mall

    shoppers’ emotions, cognition, and spending: A test of competitive

    causal theories.  Journal of Business Research,  56 (7), 529–539.

    Churchill, G., & Surprenant, C. (1982). An investigation into the

    determinants of customer satisfaction.  Journal of Marketing Research,

    19(4), 491–504.

    Colbert, F. (2003). Entrepreneurship and leadership in marketing arts.

    International Journal of Arts Management,  6 (1), 30–39.

    Cronin, J. J., & Taylor, S. A. (1992). Measuring service quality: A re-

    examination and extension.  Journal of Marketing,  56 (3), 55–68.

    Dabholkar, P. A., Shepherd, C. D., & Thorpe, D. I. (2000). A

    comprehensive framework for service quality: An investigation of 

    critical conceptual and measurement issues through a longitudinal

    study.  Journal of Retailing,  76 (2), 139–173.

    De Ruyter, K., Wetzels, M., & Bloemer, J. (1998). On the relationship

    between perceived service quality, service loyalty and switching

    costs.   International Journal of Service Industry Management,   9(5),

    436–454.

    De Ruyter, K., Wetzels, M., Lemmink, J., & Mattsson, J. (1997). The

    dynamics of the service delivery process: A value-based approach.International Journal of Research in Marketing,  14(3), 231–243.

    Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, J. (1975).  Belief, attitude, intention and behaviour:

    An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person-perception

     judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,  53(1), 53–60.

    Fornell, C. (1992). A national customer satisfaction barometer: The

    Swedish experience.  Journal of Marketing,  56 (1), 6–21.

    Frijda, N. H. (1986).   The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University

    Press.

    Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes and emotions. In M. L. En

    Haviland, & J. M. Haviland (Eds.),  Handbook of emotions. New York:

    Guildford.

    Gilmore, A., & Rentschler, R. (2002). Changes in museum management:

    A custodial or marketing emphasis?  Journal of Management Develop-

    ment,  21(10), 745–760.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    Table B1 (continued )

    Variable Question

    Pleasureb Angry/content

    Bored/entertained

    Unhappy/happy

    Disappointed/impressed

    Unhappy/joyful

    Unhappy/delighted

    Satisfactiona This is one of the best interpretation centers I could have visited

    I am pleased with my decision to visit this interpretation center

    I have really had a good time, I have had fun in this center

    I will recommend someone to visit this center

    I will say positive things about this interpretation center

    Intensificationa I have bought a book or guide for more information

    If there were a shop in this center I would buy a souvenir

    Moodb Unimpressed/enthused

    Calm/excited

    Relaxed/animated

    a

    Five points Likert scales.bScale of differential semantic.

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537 536

  • 8/15/2019 Moderacion Del Humor en La Satisfacción

    14/14

    Goulding, C. (2000). The museum environment and the visitor experience.

    European Journal of Marketing,  34(3/4), 261–278.

    Gountas, J., & Gountas, S. (2007). Personality orientations, emotional

    states, customer satisfaction, and intention to repurchase.  Journal of 

    Business Research,  60(1), 72–75.

    Gro ¨ nroos, C. (2000).   Service management and marketing. A customer

    relationship management approach (2nd ed.). West Sussex, UK: Wiley.

    Harrison, P., & Shaw, R. (2004). Consumer satisfaction and post-purchaseintentions: An exploratory study of museum visitors.   International 

    Journal of Arts Management,  6 (2), 23–33.

    Higgs, B., Polonsky, M. J., & Hollick, M. (2005). Measuring expectations.

    Forecast vs. ideal expectations. Does it really matter?   Journal of 

    Retailing and Consumer Services,  12(1), 49–64.

    Homburg, C., Koschate, N., & Hoyer, W. D. (2006). The role of cognition

    and affect in the formation of customer satisfaction: A dynamic

    perspective.  Journal of Marketing,  70(3), 21–31.

    Keaveney, S. M. (1995). Customer switching behaviour in service

    industries. An exploratory study.  Journal of Marketing,  59(2), 71–82.

    Kim, S., & Littrell, M. A. (1999). Predicting souvenir purchase intentions.

    Journal of Travel Research,  38(2), 153–162.

    LaBarbera, P. A., & Mazursky, D. (1983). A longitudinal assessment of 

    consumer satisfaction/dissatisfaction. The dynamic aspect of the

    cognitive process.  Journal of Marketing Research,  20(4), 393–404.Laros, F. J. M., & Steenkamp, J. B. E. M. (2005). Emotions in consumer

    behaviour. A hierarchical approach.   Journal of Business Research,

    58(10), 1437–1445.

    Liljander, V., & Strandvik, T. (1997). Emotions in service satisfaction.

    International Journal of Service Industry Management,  8(2), 148–169.

    Luong, A. (2005). Affective service display and customer mood.  Journal of 

    Service Research,  8(2), 117–130.

    Mano, H., & Oliver, R. L. (1993). Assessing the dimensionality and

    structure of the consumption experience: Evaluation, feeling, and

    satisfaction. Journal of Consumer Research,  20(3), 451–466.

    Mattila, A. (1998). An examination of consumers’ use of heuristic cues in

    making satisfaction judgements.   Psychology and Marketing,   15,

    477–501.

    McGill, A. L., & Iacobucci, D. (1992). The role of post-experience

    comparison standards in the evaluation of unfamiliar services.Advances in Consumer Research,  19, 570–578.

    Menon, K., & Dube ´ , L. (2000). Ensuring greater satisfaction by

    engineering salesperson response to customer emotions.   Journal of 

    Retailing,  76 (3), 285–307.

    Oatley, K. (1992).   Best laid schemes. The psychology of emotions.

    New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Oliver, R. L. (1980). A cognitive model of the antecedents and

    consequences of satisfaction decisions.  Journal of Marketing Research,

    17 (4), 460–469.

    Oliver, R. L. (1993). Cognitive, affective and attribute bases of the

    satisfaction response.  Journal of Consumer Research,  20(3), 418–430.

    Oliver, R. L. (1997).   Satisfaction. A behavioural perspective on the

    consumer. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Oliver, R. L. (1999). Whence consumer loyalty.   Journal of Marketing,

    63(4), 33–44.Oliver, R. L., & Desarbo, W. S. (1988). Response determinants in

    satisfaction judgements. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(4), 495–507.

    Oliver, R. L., Rust, R. T., & Varki, S. (1997). Customer delight.

    Foundations, findings and managerial insight.   Journal of Retailing,

    73(3), 311–336.

    Oliver, R. L., & Swan, J. E. (1989). Equity and disconfirmation

    perceptions as influences on merchant and product satisfaction.

    Journal of Consumer Research,  16 (3), 372–383.

    Olsen, S. O. (2002). Comparative evaluation and the relationship between

    quality, satisfaction, and repurchase loyalty.  Journal of the Academy of 

    Marketing Science,  30(3), 240–249.

    Olson, J. C., & Dover, P. A. (1979). Disconfirmation of consumer

    expectations through product trial.   Journal of Applied Psychology,

    64(2), 179–189.

    Patterson, P. G. (1993). Expectations and product performance as

    determinants of satisfaction for a high-involvement purchase. Psychol-

    ogy and Marketing,  10(5), 449–465.

    Phillips, D., & Baumgartner, H. (2002). The role of consumption emotions

    in the satisfaction response.   Journal of Consumer Psychology,   12(3),

    243–252.

    Ping, R. A. (1995). A parsimonious estimating technique for interaction

    and quadratic latent variables.  Journal of Marketing Research,  32(3),336–347.

    Prakash, V. (1984). Validity and reliability of the confirmation of 

    expectations paradigm as a determinant of consumer satisfaction.

    Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science,  12(4), 63–76.

    Rowley, J. (1999). Measuring total customer experience in museums.

    International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 11(6),

    303.

    Russell, J. A., & Pratt, G. A. (1980). A description of the affective quality

    attributed to environments.   Journal of Personality and Social 

    Psychology,  38(2), 311–322.

    Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion

    knowledge. Further exploration of a prototype approach.  Journal of 

    Personality and Social Psychology,  52(6), 1061–1086.

    Spreng, R. A., Mackenzie, S. B., & Olshavsky, R. W. (1996). A re-

    examination of the determinants of consumer satisfaction.  Journal of Marketing,  60(3), 15–32.

    Spreng, R. A., & Page, T. J. (2001). The impact of confidence in

    expectations on consumer satisfaction.   Psychology and Marketing,

    18(11), 1187–1204.

    Storm, C., & Storm, T. (1987). A taxonomic study of the vocabulary of 

    emotions.   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,   53(4),

    805–816.

    Szymanski, D. M., & Henard, D. H. (2001). Customer satisfaction: A

    meta-analysis of the empirical evidence.   Journal of Academy of 

    Marketing Science,  29(1), 16–35.

    Tse, D. K., & Wilton, P. C. (1988). Models of consumer satisfaction

    formation. An extension.   Journal of Marketing Research,   25(2),

    204–212.

    Vanhamme, J. (2000). The link between surprise and satisfaction: An

    exploratory research.   Journal of Marketing Management,   16 (6),565–582.

    Westbrook, R. A. (1987). Product/consumption-based affective responses

    and post-purchase processes.   Journal of Marketing Research,   24(3),

    258–270.

    Westbrook, R. A., & Oliver, R. L. (1991). The dimensionality of 

    consumption emotion patterns and consumer satisfaction.  Journal of 

    Consumer Research,  18(1), 84–91.

    White, C., & Yu, Y. T. (2005). Satisfaction emotions and consumer

    behavioral intentions.   The Journal of Services Marketing,   19(6/7),

    411–420.

    Wirtz, J., & Bateson, J. E. G. (1999). Consumer satisfaction with services:

    Integrating the environment perspective in services marketing into the

    traditional disconfirmation paradigm.   Journal of Business Research,

    44(1), 55–66.

    Wirtz, J., Mattila, A. S., & Tan, R. L. P. (2000). The moderating role of target-arousal on the impact of affect on satisfaction. An examination

    in the context of service experience.   Journal of Retailing,   76 (3),

    234–250.

    Woodruff, R. B., Cadotte, E. R., & Jenkins, R. L. (1983). Modelling

    consumer satisfaction processes using experience-based norms. Journal 

    of Marketing Research,  20(3), 296–304.

    Yuksel, A., & Yuksel, F. (2001). An updated model of travel and tourism

    purchase-consumption systems. Journal of Travel and Tourism Market-

    ing,  10(1), 3–27.

    Zeithaml, V., Berry, L., & Parasuraman, A. (1996). The behavioural

    consequences of service quality.  Journal of Marketing,  60(2), 31–46.

    Zins, A. H. (2002). Consumption emotions experience quality and

    satisfaction: A structural analysis for complainers versus no complai-

    ners. Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing,  12(2/3), 3–18.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    C. de Rojas, C. Camarero / Tourism Management 29 (2008) 525–537    537